Relationship between aging and binocular visual field in everyday chores
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.25758/set.584Keywords:
Aging, Binocular vision fields, Attentional visual fields, Visual attention, Everyday choresAbstract
Introduction – Aging can be related to autonomy loss and decline of an individual’s functional capacity impairing everyday chores execution and leading to repercussions in life quality. Purpose – Review of the current literature available regarding the repercussions of aging on the binocular and attentional Visual Field and the influence of the binocular visual field in the elderly, regarding reading, writing, and walking. Methodology – This study is a meta-analysis, based on a review of the contents of 37 scientific articles, which were subsequently organized in an observation grid and a comparative chart. Outcomes – In 32.43% (n=12) of the articles authors state that it is possible that aging diminishes the extension of the binocular and attentional visual field. Repercussions of the decrease of the binocular visual field extension, without attentional factor on daily activities, are referred to in 54.05% (n=20) of the articles. Among those, 40.53% (n=15) established a relationship between the binocular visual field and the performance in reading, writing, or walking. In 45.95% (n=17) of the articles, the authors related binocular visual field with attentional factor, and in 10.81% (n=4) of them established the same connection. Discussion/Findings – Aging causes a decrease in the visual field, particularly in peripheral vision. This decrease impacts reading, writing, and walking capacities, which can be greatly affected in the presence of diminished visual attention.
Downloads
References
Cancela D. O processo de envelhecimento [Internet]. Porto: D. Cancela; 2007 [cited 2007 May 15]. Available from: http://www.psicologia.com.pt/artigos/textos/TL0097.pdf. Portuguese
Instituto Nacional de Estatística. Censos 2001 [Internet]. Lisboa: INE; 2001 [cited 2002 Oct 21]. Available from: http://paginas.ispgaya.pt/~vmca/Documentos_links/censo2001.pdf. Portuguese
Carrilho MJ, Gonçalves C. Dinâmicas territoriais do envelhecimento: análise exploratória dos resultados dos Censos 91 e 2001 [Territorial dynamics of population ageing: an exploratory analysis of the results of the 1991 and 2001 Census]. Rev Estudos Demográficos. 2004;(36):175. Portuguese
West SK, Munoz B, Rubin GS, Schein OD, Bandeen-Roche K, Zeger S, et al. Function and visual impairment in a population-based study of older adults: the SEE project – Salisbury Eye Evaluation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38(1):72-82.
Patel I, Turano KA, Broman AT, Bandeen-Roche K, Muñoz B, West K. Measures of visual function and percentage of preferred walking speed in older adults: the Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47(1):65-71.
Laukkaken P, Heikkinen E, Kauppinen M. Muscle strength and mobility as predictors of survival in 75-84-year-old-people. Age Aging. 1995;24(6):468-73.
Harrington DO, Drake MV, editors. The visual fields: a textbook and atlas of clinical perimetry. St Louis: Mosby; 1990.
Weijland A, Fankhauser F, Bebie H, Flammer J, editors. Automated perimetry: visual field digest. 5th ed. Koniz/Bern, Switzerland: Haag-Streit AG; 2004. ISBN 9783033001084
Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs DS. Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5(12):2210-9.
Johnson CA, Adams AJ, Lewis RA. Evidence for a neural basis of age-related visual field loss in normal observers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1989;30(9):2056-64.
Jaffe GJ, Alvarado JA, Juster RP. Age-related changes of the normal visual field. Arch Ophthalmol. 1986;104(7):1021-5.
Nelson-Quigg JM, Cello K, Johnson CA. Predicting binocular visual field sensitivity from monocular visual field results. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41(8):2212-21.
Hassan SE, Turano KA, Muñoz B, Munro C, Roche KB, West SK. Cognitive and vision loss affects the topography of the attentional visual field. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49(10):4672-8.
Wakayama A, Matsumoto C, Iwagaki A, Otori T. Binocular summation within the binocular visual field. In Wall M, Wild JM, editors. Proceedings of the XIIIth International Perimetric Society Meeting, Gardone Riviera (Italy), September 6-9, 1998. Perimetry Update. 1998/1999. p. 193-200.
McCalley LT, Bouwhuis DG, Joula JF. Age changes in the distribution of visual attention. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1995;50(6):P316-31.
Serences JT, Yantis S, Culberson A, Awh E. Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92(6):3538-45.
Madden DJ. Aging and visual attention. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16(2):70-4.
Wang DY, Enstsminger S. Age and attentional capacity. In Proceedings of Fifth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design. 2007. p. 427-32.
Green CS, Bavelier D. Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature. 2003;423(6939):534-7.
Coeckelbergh TR, Cornelissen FW, Brouwer WH, Kooijman AC. Age-related changes in the functional visual field: further evidence for an inverse Age x Eccentricity effect. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2004;59(1):P11-8.
Kosnik W, Winslow L, Kline D, Rasinski K, Sekuler R. Visual changes in daily life throughout adulthood. J Gerontol. 1988;43(3):P63-70.
Vargas-Martín F, Peli E. Eye movements of patients with tunnel vision while walking. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47(12):5295-302.
Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Brabyn JA. Seeing into old age: vision function beyond acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 1999;76(3):141-58.
Ramrattan RS, Wolfs RC, Panda-Jonas S, Jonas JB, Bakker D, Pols HA, Hofman A, de Jong PT. Prevalence and causes of visual field loss in the elderly and associations with impairment in daily functioning. Arch Ophthalmol. 2001;119:1788-94.
Schieber F, Gilland J. Age differences in the useful field of view during real-world driving. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annual Meeting. 2005;49(2):182-5.
Hartwerth RS, Wheat JL, Rangaswamy NV. Age-related losses of retinal ganglion cells and axons. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49(10):4437-43.
Vicente Pérez G, editor. Atlas de perimetria computorizada. Barcelona: I. M. & C.; s.d.
Turano KA, Broman AT, Bandeen-Roche K, Muñoz B, Rubin GS, West SK. Association of visual field loss and mobility performance in older adults: Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study. Optom Vis Sci. 2004;81(5):298-307.
Freeman EE, Muñoz B, Rubin G, West SK. Visual field loss increases the risk of falls in older adults: the Salisbury Eye Evaluation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48(10):4445-50.
Yu D, Legge GE, Park H, Gage E, Chung ST. Development of a training protocol to improve reading performance in peripheral vision. Vision Res. 2010;50(1):36-45.
Timmis MA, Bennett SJ, Buckley JG. Visuomotor control of step descent: evidence of specialised role of the lower visual field. Exp Brain Res. 2009;195(2):219-27.
Marigold DS, Patla AE. Visual information from the lower visual field is important for walking across multi-surface terrain. Exp Brain Res. 2008;188(1):23-31.
Yao JG, Gao X, Yan HM, Li CY. Field of attention for instantaneous object recognition. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(1):e16343.
Lee HW, Kwon M, Legge GE, Gefroh JJ. Training improves reading speed in peripheral vision: is it due to attention? J Vision. 2010;10(6):18.
Coleman AL. Sources of binocular suprathreshold visual field loss in a cohort of older women being followed for risk of falls. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc. 2007;105:312-29.
Rayner K, Well AD, Pollatsek A. Asymmetry of the effective visual field in reading. Percept Psychophys. 1980;27(6):537-44.
Di Fabio RP, Zampieri C, Henke J, Olson K, Rickheim D, Russell M. Influence of elderly executive cognitive function on attention in the lower visual field during step initiation. Gerontology. 2005;51(2):94-107.
Downloads
Published
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2022 Saúde e Tecnologia
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
The journal Saúde & Tecnologia offers immediate free access to its content, following the principle that making scientific knowledge available to the public free of charge provides greater worldwide democratization of knowledge.
The journal Saúde & Tecnologia does not charge authors any submission or article processing charges (APC).
All content is licensed under a Creative Commons CC-BY-NC-ND license. Authors have the right to: reproduce their work in physical or digital form for personal, professional, or teaching use, but not for commercial use (including the sale of the right to access the article); deposit on their website, that of their institution or in a repository an exact copy in electronic format of the article published by Saúde & Tecnologia, provided that reference is made to its publication in Saúde & Tecnologia and its content (including symbols identifying the journal) is not altered; publish in a book of which they are authors or editors the total or partial content of the manuscript, provided that reference is made to its publication in Saúde & Tecnologia.